RESEARCHERS GAIN INSIGHT INTO WHY BRAIN AREAS FAIL TO WORK TOGETHER IN AUTISM

[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]

 



U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 
NATIONAL INSTITUTES OF HEALTH 
NIH News 
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) 
http://www.nichd.nih.gov/ 

FOR IMMEDIATE RELEASE: Wednesday, July 12, 2006

CONTACT: Robert Bock or Marianne Glass Miller, 301-496-5133,
bockr@xxxxxxxxxxxx 

RESEARCHERS GAIN INSIGHT INTO WHY BRAIN AREAS FAIL TO WORK TOGETHER IN
AUTISM
Basis For Why People With Autism Think In Pictures

Researchers have found in two studies that autism may involve a lack of
connections and coordination in separate areas of the brain.

In people with autism, the brain areas that perform complex analysis
appear less likely to work together during problem solving tasks than in
people who do not have the disorder, report researchers working in a
network funded by the National Institutes of Health. The researchers
found that communications between these higher-order centers in the
brains of people with autism appear to be directly related to the
thickness of the anatomical connections between them.

In a separate report, the same research team found that, in people with
autism, brain areas normally associated with visual tasks also appear to
be active during language-related tasks, providing evidence to explain a
bias towards visual thinking common in autism.

"These findings provide support to a new theory that views autism as a
failure of brain regions to communicate with each other," said Duane
Alexander, M.D., Director of NIH's National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development. "The findings may one day provide the basis for
improved treatments for autism that stimulate communication between
brain areas."

The studies and the theory are the work of Marcel Just, Ph.D., D.O. Hebb
Professor of Psychology at Carnegie Mellon University in Pittsburgh,
Pennsylvania, and Nancy Minshew, M.D., Professor of Psychiatry and
Neurology at the University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine and their
colleagues.

The research was conducted by the Collaborative Program of Excellence in
Autism, a research network funded by the NICHD and the National
Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders.

People with autism often have difficulty communicating and interacting
socially with other people. The saying "unable to see the forest for the
trees" describes how people with autism frequently excel at details, yet
struggle to comprehend the larger picture. For example, some children
with autism may become spelling bee champions, but have difficulty
understanding the meaning of a sentence or a story.

An earlier finding by these researchers described how a group of people
with autism tended to use parts of the brain typically associated with
processing shapes to remember letters of the alphabet. A news release
detailing that finding appears at
http://www.nichd.nih.gov/new/releases/final_autism.cfm.

Participants with autism in both current studies had normal I.Q. There
were no significant differences between the participants with and
without autism in age or I.Q.

The first of the two new studies recently was published online in the
journal "Cerebral Cortex". In that study, the researchers used a brain
imaging technique known as functional magnetic resonance imaging, or
fMRI, to view the brains of people with autism as well as a comparison
group of people who do not have autism. All of the study participants
were asked to complete the "Tower of London" test. The task involves
moving three balls into a specified arrangement in an array of three
receptacles. The "Tower of London" is used to gauge the functioning of
the prefrontal cortex.

This brain area, located in the front, upper part of the brain, deals
with strategic planning and problem-solving. The prefrontal cortex is
the executive area of the brain, in which decision making, judgment, and
impulse control reside.

A little further back is the parietal cortex, which controls high-level
visual thinking and visual imagery, supporting the visual aspects of the
problem-solving. Both the prefrontal and parietal cortex play a critical
part in performing the "Tower of London" test.

In the normal participants, the prefrontal cortex and the parietal
cortex tended to function in synchrony (increasing and decreasing their
activity at the same time) while solving the Tower of London task. This
suggests that the two brain areas were working together to solve the
problem.

In the participants with autism, however, the two brain areas,
prefrontal and parietal, were less likely to function in synchrony while
working on the task.

The researchers made another discovery, for the first time finding a
relationship between this lower level of synchrony and the properties of
some of the neurological "cables" or white matter fiber tracts that
connect brain areas.

White matter consists of fibers that, like cabling, connect brain areas.
The largest of the white matter tracts is known as the corpus callosum,
which allows communication between the two hemispheres (halves) of the
brain.

"The size of the corpus callosum was smaller in the group with autism,
suggesting that inter-regional brain cabling is disrupted in autism,"
Dr. Just said.

In essence, the extent to which the two key brain areas (prefrontal and
parietal) of the autistic participants worked in synchrony was
correlated with the size of the corpus callosum. The smaller the corpus
callosum, the less likely the two areas were to function in synchrony.
In the normal participants, however, the size of the corpus callosum did
not appear to be correlated with the ability of the two areas to work in
synchrony.

"This finding provides strong evidence that autism is a disorder
involving the biological connections and the coordination of processing
between brain areas," Dr. Just said.

He added, however, that the thickness, or extent, of connections between
brain areas may not be the basis for the disorder. Although the
neurological connections between the prefrontal cortex appear to be
reduced in autism, the brains of people with autism have thicker
connections between certain brain regions within each hemisphere.

"At this point, we can say that autism appears to be a disorder of
abnormal neurological and informational connections of the brain, but we
can't yet explain the nature of that abnormality," Dr. Just said.

In the second study, published online in the journal "Brain", the
researchers examined the extent to which brain areas involved in
language interact with brain regions that process images. Dr. Just
explained that earlier studies, as well as anecdotal accounts, suggest
that people with autism rely more heavily on visual and spatial areas of
the brain than do other people.

In this study, the researchers used fMRI to examine brain functioning in
participants with autism and in normal participants during a true-false
test involving reading sentences with low imagery content and high
imagery content. A typical low imagery sentence consisted of
constructions like "Addition, subtraction, and multiplication are all
math skills." A high imagery sentence, "The number eight when rotated 90
degrees looks like a pair of eyeglasses," would first activate left
prefrontal brain areas involved with language, and then would involve
parietal areas dealing with vision and imagery as the study participant
mentally manipulated the number eight.

As the researchers expected, the visual brain areas of the normal
participants were active only when evaluating sentences with imagery
content. In contrast, the visual centers in the brains of participants
with autism were active when evaluating both high imagery and low
imagery sentences.

"The heavy reliance on visualization in people with autism may be an
adaptation to compensate for a diminished ability to call on prefrontal
regions of the brain," Dr. Just said.

The second study also confirmed the observations in the first study --
that the prefrontal and parietal brain regions of the cortex in people
with autism were less likely to work in synchrony than were the brains
of normal volunteers. The second study also confirmed that the extent to
which the two parts of the cortex could work together was correlated
with the size of the corpus callosum that connected them.

Dr. Just and his colleagues are conducting additional studies to
ascertain the nature of the abnormality of the connections in the brains
of people with autism.

The NICHD sponsors research on development, before and after birth;
maternal, child, and family health; reproductive biology and population
issues; and medical rehabilitation. For more information, visit the
Institute's Web site at http://www.nichd.nih.gov/. 

The National Institutes of Health (NIH) -- "The Nation's Medical
Research Agency" -- includes 27 Institutes and Centers and is a
component of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. It is the
primary federal agency for conducting and supporting basic, clinical and
translational medical research, and it investigates the causes,
treatments, and cures for both common and rare diseases. For more
information about NIH and its programs, visit www.nih.gov.
  
##
 
This NIH News Release is available online at:
http://www.nih.gov/news/pr/jul2006/nichd-12.htm.

To subscribe (or unsubscribe) from this list, go to
http://list.nih.gov/cgi-bin/wa?SUBED1=nihpress&A=1.

[Index of Archives]     [CDC News]     [FDA News]     [USDA News]     [Yosemite News]     [Steve's Art]     [PhotoForum]     [SB Lupus]     [STB]

  Powered by Linux